Since entering graduate school almost
two years ago, I have often reflected that I can feel myself growing smarter. I
am not only talking about learning new facts and applying them to my practice
as an educator. More, I am referring specifically to my ability to think
critically, reason, question, and problem solve. Many times I thought I was
just excited to be back in the classroom in that capacity again. However, after
reading this week’s text selection, I felt validated when Slavin (2012)
discussed brain research that found “that as a person gains in knowledge and
skill, his or her brain becomes more efficient” (p. 155). I understand that
this specific research supports the idea
that our brain utilizes fewer spaces as skill increases; however, this ties
neatly with the notion of automaticity, which occurs when skills are so fluid
and fluent that there is no apparent effort involved. I am by no means implying
that I find graduate school effortless. To the contrary, being in graduate
school, working full time, and having a life provides numerous challenges that
sometimes seem insurmountable. Instead, I’m referring to the ease at which I
accomplish the tasks put forth to me by my professors.
Thus, the great hope as a teacher is to identify ways to
instruct students, allow them to discover, and practice new skills to the point
where eventually there is that level of automaticity. So the great question, is
how do we teachers attain that? Based on readings, some of that comes from the
brain growing, developing, and doing its things. However, this seems a very
minor component of the larger puzzle. Exposure is essential to developing the
proper pathways to build knowledge (Slavin, 2012). Therefore, adults (including
parents and teachers) must provide rich and meaningful experiences to help
build the neurological pathways and background knowledge to facilitate long-term
storage of new semantic memories.
A second takeaway from this week’s readings also caused me
to reflect on my personal experiences as a student. The notion of interference
was raised by Slavin (2012), and was defined as “the inhibition of recall of
certain information by the presence of other information in memory” (158). What’s
interesting about this is pondering the number of stimuli that could
potentially interfere with the acquisition of new memory. Personally, I’m
fascinated by what makes for optimal conditions to learning and how to
subsequently apply that understanding to help my students learn. If a student
is being bombarded by auditory stimuli, how can there be merit in many student’s
claims that they learn better why listening to music – and not the calm,
tranquil kind. This seems counterintuitive, but so many claim this, there must
be some merit there, right? Certainly, I look forward to learning more on this
subject and how I can apply this to my practice.
Slavin, R. E. (2012). Educational
psychology: Theory and practice (Tenth ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson.
You think like a scientist. I think it's a unique and invaluable quality in an educator -- that drive to not only learn for yourself, but to see what will truly trigger learning in your students.
ReplyDelete